English minimal pair words by syllabification












2















Are there English minimal pairs created by different syllabification, specifically of lexical words?










share|improve this question




















  • 2





    Sure; nitrate, night rate, and Nye trait, for instance.

    – John Lawler
    Mar 19 at 2:17











  • Night rate and Nye trait are phrases, not words

    – GJC
    Mar 19 at 2:25






  • 1





    Only according to English spelling. You'd be hard pressed to find phonetic criteria that distinguish them.

    – John Lawler
    Mar 19 at 2:26











  • And I scream versus ice cream is a famous example. (I scream, you scream, we all scream for ice cream.) But it's not a single word.

    – Peter Shor
    Mar 19 at 2:55








  • 1





    Oops. I misread. But anyway, what's a lexical word?

    – Mitch
    2 days ago
















2















Are there English minimal pairs created by different syllabification, specifically of lexical words?










share|improve this question




















  • 2





    Sure; nitrate, night rate, and Nye trait, for instance.

    – John Lawler
    Mar 19 at 2:17











  • Night rate and Nye trait are phrases, not words

    – GJC
    Mar 19 at 2:25






  • 1





    Only according to English spelling. You'd be hard pressed to find phonetic criteria that distinguish them.

    – John Lawler
    Mar 19 at 2:26











  • And I scream versus ice cream is a famous example. (I scream, you scream, we all scream for ice cream.) But it's not a single word.

    – Peter Shor
    Mar 19 at 2:55








  • 1





    Oops. I misread. But anyway, what's a lexical word?

    – Mitch
    2 days ago














2












2








2


1






Are there English minimal pairs created by different syllabification, specifically of lexical words?










share|improve this question
















Are there English minimal pairs created by different syllabification, specifically of lexical words?







phonology phonetics syllables lexicon minimal-pairs






share|improve this question















share|improve this question













share|improve this question




share|improve this question








edited Mar 19 at 2:32







GJC

















asked Mar 19 at 1:55









GJCGJC

340212




340212








  • 2





    Sure; nitrate, night rate, and Nye trait, for instance.

    – John Lawler
    Mar 19 at 2:17











  • Night rate and Nye trait are phrases, not words

    – GJC
    Mar 19 at 2:25






  • 1





    Only according to English spelling. You'd be hard pressed to find phonetic criteria that distinguish them.

    – John Lawler
    Mar 19 at 2:26











  • And I scream versus ice cream is a famous example. (I scream, you scream, we all scream for ice cream.) But it's not a single word.

    – Peter Shor
    Mar 19 at 2:55








  • 1





    Oops. I misread. But anyway, what's a lexical word?

    – Mitch
    2 days ago














  • 2





    Sure; nitrate, night rate, and Nye trait, for instance.

    – John Lawler
    Mar 19 at 2:17











  • Night rate and Nye trait are phrases, not words

    – GJC
    Mar 19 at 2:25






  • 1





    Only according to English spelling. You'd be hard pressed to find phonetic criteria that distinguish them.

    – John Lawler
    Mar 19 at 2:26











  • And I scream versus ice cream is a famous example. (I scream, you scream, we all scream for ice cream.) But it's not a single word.

    – Peter Shor
    Mar 19 at 2:55








  • 1





    Oops. I misread. But anyway, what's a lexical word?

    – Mitch
    2 days ago








2




2





Sure; nitrate, night rate, and Nye trait, for instance.

– John Lawler
Mar 19 at 2:17





Sure; nitrate, night rate, and Nye trait, for instance.

– John Lawler
Mar 19 at 2:17













Night rate and Nye trait are phrases, not words

– GJC
Mar 19 at 2:25





Night rate and Nye trait are phrases, not words

– GJC
Mar 19 at 2:25




1




1





Only according to English spelling. You'd be hard pressed to find phonetic criteria that distinguish them.

– John Lawler
Mar 19 at 2:26





Only according to English spelling. You'd be hard pressed to find phonetic criteria that distinguish them.

– John Lawler
Mar 19 at 2:26













And I scream versus ice cream is a famous example. (I scream, you scream, we all scream for ice cream.) But it's not a single word.

– Peter Shor
Mar 19 at 2:55







And I scream versus ice cream is a famous example. (I scream, you scream, we all scream for ice cream.) But it's not a single word.

– Peter Shor
Mar 19 at 2:55






1




1





Oops. I misread. But anyway, what's a lexical word?

– Mitch
2 days ago





Oops. I misread. But anyway, what's a lexical word?

– Mitch
2 days ago










4 Answers
4






active

oldest

votes


















2














You are asking after minimal pairs created through juncture, or the transition between two phonemes. Changing juncture would effectively change the syllabification of a word, since it would change whether adjacent sounds are part of the same syllable or are part of two adjacent syllables.



There are lots of examples of juncture generating minimal pair phrases. A generative one is to play with the articles a (before a consonant) and an (before a noun):




an aim / a name



an ad / a nad



an app / a nap




Notice how the /n/ changes slightly in emphasis between these examples but the vowel stays the same. The only difference is an allophonic change - the consonant /n/ is more voiced when it starts the word. Junctures are created through these allophonic changes.



The problem with finding a word-level minimal pair is suggested in this Wikipedia article on minimal pairs. English juncture produces semantic differences primarily between words, and those junctures are created by allophonic shifts, or subtle shifts in how we pronounce phonemes. Within words in English allophonic changes seldom present more than an accent difference. Imagine someone saying cat simple (/kæt/) versus with an aspirated t (/[kætʰ/) and having that produce a different word. It's just a cat that sounds funny! So if the allophonic sound differences don't generate new words, then syllabic breakdown within a single word doesn't generate another single word.



Finally, exceptions are hard to find because many near-homophones that seem to qualify (like catastrophe -> cat ass trophy) involve vowel shifts (from /kə/ to /kæ*/ or /ka*/ that make it no longer a minimal pair based on juncture alone. The "co-ring -> coring" example mentioned above also fits this paradigm: the /ko/ of "co-ring" is shifting to the /kɔ/ or /kə/ of "coring." So we're not just looking at a syllabic shift but a vowel shift. That said, the absence of evidence is not the evidence of absence, so it's theoretically possible two words could be juncture-based minimal pairs.






share|improve this answer































    2














    If you include compound words in your definition of "lexical word", this should be possible in theory, but I can't think of a specific actual example.



    Here's a hypothetical example: you could have a pair like sauce.pan vs. "saw.span", where the first would tend to have a phonetically shorter vowel in the initial syllable, while the second would have a less aspirated plosive in the onset of the final syllable.



    In John Wells' article on "Syllabification and allophony", he gives the near-minimal pair of selfish vs. shellfish, and argues that the first is self.ish while the second is shell.fish. Aside from the difference in the first consonant, I think that some people might argue that "selfish" is actually syllabified as "sel.fish" and that the difference in pronunciation relative to "shell.fish" is due to the absence of tertiary stress on the last syllable (e.g. ˈshellˌfish vs. ˈsel.fish). Wells doesn't think that tertiary stress is necessary, but other linguists do make use of the concept. That's why I think the clearest example would consist of a pair of compound words. Wells also mentions the near-minimal pair of "toe-strap" vs. "toast-rack".



    Some theories have more restrictive rules about syllabification



    Wells' conception of syllabification isn't universally accepted. In some theories, the rule of "maximizing the onset" of a syllable is treated as a generally applicable rule, even across morpheme boundaries in inflected words (such as planning, which some linguists would syllabify as /ˈplæ.nɪŋ/, despite the fact that it is a form of the word plan /plæn/).



    In such theories, there are several possible ways to analyze the distinction between pairs like "coring" and "co-ring" mentioned by Chris H. (In some accents of American English, both of these words can be analyzed as having the vowel /o/ as the nucleus of the first syllable.)



    The answers by user340953 and TaliesinMerlin mention the concept of "juncture", which is basically the idea that word (or morpheme) boundaries correspond to some kind of phonological entity. My impression is that this is not a very popular term/concept in more recent theories; user6726's answer to this Linguistics SE question says that "the concept has fallen into desuetude in linguistics over the past half century".



    The concept that I've seen used more often in papers that I've come across that take the maximizing-onsets approach to syllabification is to appeal to a larger phonological unit than the syllable, the "foot". "Feet" are mostly related to stress; to simplify things, you can think of each stressed syllable in an English word as being the start of a "foot".



    We could say that despite containing the same syllables, /ko/ and /rɪŋ/, "coring" and "co-ring" contrast because the second syllable of "co-ring" is (or at least was originally) a foot, but the second syllable of "coring" is not a foot. (The exact division of words into feet seems to be somewhat controversial; I'm not sure whether it would be more better to analyze "coring" from "core" as (ko.rɪŋ), with a disyllabic Hσ foot, or as (ko)rɪŋ, with a monosyllabic H foot followed by an unfooted syllable. Feet are commonly said to be "binary at some level of analysis" (either bimoraic or bisyllabic) which permits (ko.rɪŋ), and I think that would be the more usual foot-based analysis, but I think some people may have proposed more restrictive definitions of feet.)






    share|improve this answer

































      1














      There are homonyms where one word is formed from a stem+prefix and the other isn't. The stress is often different, or the first/prefix vowel sound. One of the closest, at least in some accents, is coring (wiktionary). The more familiar meaning is from core+ing: "cutting a core" or (n) the core so formed, but there's a mathematical meaning from co+ring: "The dual of a ring".






      share|improve this answer
























      • As you mentioned, the stress in such words is often different, so I think it isn't a conclusive minimal pair for syllabification. Coring vs. co-ring could be analyzed as /ˈko.rɪŋ/ vs. /ˈkoˌrɪŋ/.

        – sumelic
        2 days ago











      • @sumelic yes, I think there's value in the approach but the example could be improved upon. I also have to take time to think about your transcription, as I'm no expert on the finer points.

        – Chris H
        2 days ago






      • 2





        The issue that I'm thinking of is mainly theoretical. Intuitively, I wouldn't say that the first syllable of "coring" (from "core") is /ko/, but I also wouldn't say that the first syllable of "planning" is /plæ/, or that the first syllable of "lemon" is /lɛ/. However, syllabifications like "le.mon" are fairly often given, and justified by reference to the principle of "maximizing the onset" of syllables.

        – sumelic
        2 days ago











      • I mention this in my answer, but the problem is that the difference in sound between the two words goes beyond syllabification. The vowel shifts. I think your idea is quite good (prefixes and suffixes may well generate the subtle allophonic difference that would signal a shift in juncture), but we're in a different kind of minimal pair with coring.

        – TaliesinMerlin
        2 days ago













      • @TaliesinMerlin: In certain varieties of American English, both "core" and "co-" can be analyzed as containing the same vowel phoneme, /o/.

        – sumelic
        2 days ago



















      0














      I don't think it is possible in English, since, unless the actual phonemes themselves change, as discussed in other answers, then the words would be pronounced the same. This is because we tend to pronounce syllable boundaries according to the rules of phonotactics, not according to the morphemic boundaries.



      So if nitrate, night rate and nigh trait were each one word, and if the phonemes themselves were the same in each case, then they would simply sound the same, despite three different morphemic boundaries:




      nitr|ate

      night|rate

      nigh|trait




      where | represents the morphemic boundary.



      I think we have to have to go to a language like Irish, which does something you don't see in English, to get two words that differ in morphemic boundary, but the problem is they sound the same!



      This isn't quite what you are looking for but I think it is the nearest you will get.



      Some words than historically ended in an -n now cause the following word to modify instead. This is usually called "eclipsis" but nasalisation is the correct term for the grammatical process, with "eclipsis" referring to the orthographic convention. It is described here. For anyone with Welsh (or similar) it is the nasal mutation with the addition of a rather strange orthography.



      So from ár (treated as from *árn) "our" and athair "father", we get ár nathair /aːr nahǝr/ "our father". Unfortnately, nathair also means "snake", so ár nathair can also mean "our snake", thus confusing everyone who has ever learnt the Lord's prayer in Irish. Thus nathair is a pair of homonymous words, caused by a movement in the morphemic boundary.



      They sound the same and look the same, unless you use capitals, as in the Lord's Prayer in Irish, in which case you get




      Ár nAthair vs Ár Nathair




      I know this is not exactly what was asked for but I don't think you will find anything better. Of course it is as much to with orthographic convention as anything else. This is illustrated by switching to Scots Gaelic, where we get




      Ar n-Athair vs Ar Nathair.







      share|improve this answer























        Your Answer








        StackExchange.ready(function() {
        var channelOptions = {
        tags: "".split(" "),
        id: "97"
        };
        initTagRenderer("".split(" "), "".split(" "), channelOptions);

        StackExchange.using("externalEditor", function() {
        // Have to fire editor after snippets, if snippets enabled
        if (StackExchange.settings.snippets.snippetsEnabled) {
        StackExchange.using("snippets", function() {
        createEditor();
        });
        }
        else {
        createEditor();
        }
        });

        function createEditor() {
        StackExchange.prepareEditor({
        heartbeatType: 'answer',
        autoActivateHeartbeat: false,
        convertImagesToLinks: false,
        noModals: true,
        showLowRepImageUploadWarning: true,
        reputationToPostImages: null,
        bindNavPrevention: true,
        postfix: "",
        imageUploader: {
        brandingHtml: "Powered by u003ca class="icon-imgur-white" href="https://imgur.com/"u003eu003c/au003e",
        contentPolicyHtml: "User contributions licensed under u003ca href="https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-sa/3.0/"u003ecc by-sa 3.0 with attribution requiredu003c/au003e u003ca href="https://stackoverflow.com/legal/content-policy"u003e(content policy)u003c/au003e",
        allowUrls: true
        },
        noCode: true, onDemand: true,
        discardSelector: ".discard-answer"
        ,immediatelyShowMarkdownHelp:true
        });


        }
        });














        draft saved

        draft discarded


















        StackExchange.ready(
        function () {
        StackExchange.openid.initPostLogin('.new-post-login', 'https%3a%2f%2fenglish.stackexchange.com%2fquestions%2f490330%2fenglish-minimal-pair-words-by-syllabification%23new-answer', 'question_page');
        }
        );

        Post as a guest















        Required, but never shown

























        4 Answers
        4






        active

        oldest

        votes








        4 Answers
        4






        active

        oldest

        votes









        active

        oldest

        votes






        active

        oldest

        votes









        2














        You are asking after minimal pairs created through juncture, or the transition between two phonemes. Changing juncture would effectively change the syllabification of a word, since it would change whether adjacent sounds are part of the same syllable or are part of two adjacent syllables.



        There are lots of examples of juncture generating minimal pair phrases. A generative one is to play with the articles a (before a consonant) and an (before a noun):




        an aim / a name



        an ad / a nad



        an app / a nap




        Notice how the /n/ changes slightly in emphasis between these examples but the vowel stays the same. The only difference is an allophonic change - the consonant /n/ is more voiced when it starts the word. Junctures are created through these allophonic changes.



        The problem with finding a word-level minimal pair is suggested in this Wikipedia article on minimal pairs. English juncture produces semantic differences primarily between words, and those junctures are created by allophonic shifts, or subtle shifts in how we pronounce phonemes. Within words in English allophonic changes seldom present more than an accent difference. Imagine someone saying cat simple (/kæt/) versus with an aspirated t (/[kætʰ/) and having that produce a different word. It's just a cat that sounds funny! So if the allophonic sound differences don't generate new words, then syllabic breakdown within a single word doesn't generate another single word.



        Finally, exceptions are hard to find because many near-homophones that seem to qualify (like catastrophe -> cat ass trophy) involve vowel shifts (from /kə/ to /kæ*/ or /ka*/ that make it no longer a minimal pair based on juncture alone. The "co-ring -> coring" example mentioned above also fits this paradigm: the /ko/ of "co-ring" is shifting to the /kɔ/ or /kə/ of "coring." So we're not just looking at a syllabic shift but a vowel shift. That said, the absence of evidence is not the evidence of absence, so it's theoretically possible two words could be juncture-based minimal pairs.






        share|improve this answer




























          2














          You are asking after minimal pairs created through juncture, or the transition between two phonemes. Changing juncture would effectively change the syllabification of a word, since it would change whether adjacent sounds are part of the same syllable or are part of two adjacent syllables.



          There are lots of examples of juncture generating minimal pair phrases. A generative one is to play with the articles a (before a consonant) and an (before a noun):




          an aim / a name



          an ad / a nad



          an app / a nap




          Notice how the /n/ changes slightly in emphasis between these examples but the vowel stays the same. The only difference is an allophonic change - the consonant /n/ is more voiced when it starts the word. Junctures are created through these allophonic changes.



          The problem with finding a word-level minimal pair is suggested in this Wikipedia article on minimal pairs. English juncture produces semantic differences primarily between words, and those junctures are created by allophonic shifts, or subtle shifts in how we pronounce phonemes. Within words in English allophonic changes seldom present more than an accent difference. Imagine someone saying cat simple (/kæt/) versus with an aspirated t (/[kætʰ/) and having that produce a different word. It's just a cat that sounds funny! So if the allophonic sound differences don't generate new words, then syllabic breakdown within a single word doesn't generate another single word.



          Finally, exceptions are hard to find because many near-homophones that seem to qualify (like catastrophe -> cat ass trophy) involve vowel shifts (from /kə/ to /kæ*/ or /ka*/ that make it no longer a minimal pair based on juncture alone. The "co-ring -> coring" example mentioned above also fits this paradigm: the /ko/ of "co-ring" is shifting to the /kɔ/ or /kə/ of "coring." So we're not just looking at a syllabic shift but a vowel shift. That said, the absence of evidence is not the evidence of absence, so it's theoretically possible two words could be juncture-based minimal pairs.






          share|improve this answer


























            2












            2








            2







            You are asking after minimal pairs created through juncture, or the transition between two phonemes. Changing juncture would effectively change the syllabification of a word, since it would change whether adjacent sounds are part of the same syllable or are part of two adjacent syllables.



            There are lots of examples of juncture generating minimal pair phrases. A generative one is to play with the articles a (before a consonant) and an (before a noun):




            an aim / a name



            an ad / a nad



            an app / a nap




            Notice how the /n/ changes slightly in emphasis between these examples but the vowel stays the same. The only difference is an allophonic change - the consonant /n/ is more voiced when it starts the word. Junctures are created through these allophonic changes.



            The problem with finding a word-level minimal pair is suggested in this Wikipedia article on minimal pairs. English juncture produces semantic differences primarily between words, and those junctures are created by allophonic shifts, or subtle shifts in how we pronounce phonemes. Within words in English allophonic changes seldom present more than an accent difference. Imagine someone saying cat simple (/kæt/) versus with an aspirated t (/[kætʰ/) and having that produce a different word. It's just a cat that sounds funny! So if the allophonic sound differences don't generate new words, then syllabic breakdown within a single word doesn't generate another single word.



            Finally, exceptions are hard to find because many near-homophones that seem to qualify (like catastrophe -> cat ass trophy) involve vowel shifts (from /kə/ to /kæ*/ or /ka*/ that make it no longer a minimal pair based on juncture alone. The "co-ring -> coring" example mentioned above also fits this paradigm: the /ko/ of "co-ring" is shifting to the /kɔ/ or /kə/ of "coring." So we're not just looking at a syllabic shift but a vowel shift. That said, the absence of evidence is not the evidence of absence, so it's theoretically possible two words could be juncture-based minimal pairs.






            share|improve this answer













            You are asking after minimal pairs created through juncture, or the transition between two phonemes. Changing juncture would effectively change the syllabification of a word, since it would change whether adjacent sounds are part of the same syllable or are part of two adjacent syllables.



            There are lots of examples of juncture generating minimal pair phrases. A generative one is to play with the articles a (before a consonant) and an (before a noun):




            an aim / a name



            an ad / a nad



            an app / a nap




            Notice how the /n/ changes slightly in emphasis between these examples but the vowel stays the same. The only difference is an allophonic change - the consonant /n/ is more voiced when it starts the word. Junctures are created through these allophonic changes.



            The problem with finding a word-level minimal pair is suggested in this Wikipedia article on minimal pairs. English juncture produces semantic differences primarily between words, and those junctures are created by allophonic shifts, or subtle shifts in how we pronounce phonemes. Within words in English allophonic changes seldom present more than an accent difference. Imagine someone saying cat simple (/kæt/) versus with an aspirated t (/[kætʰ/) and having that produce a different word. It's just a cat that sounds funny! So if the allophonic sound differences don't generate new words, then syllabic breakdown within a single word doesn't generate another single word.



            Finally, exceptions are hard to find because many near-homophones that seem to qualify (like catastrophe -> cat ass trophy) involve vowel shifts (from /kə/ to /kæ*/ or /ka*/ that make it no longer a minimal pair based on juncture alone. The "co-ring -> coring" example mentioned above also fits this paradigm: the /ko/ of "co-ring" is shifting to the /kɔ/ or /kə/ of "coring." So we're not just looking at a syllabic shift but a vowel shift. That said, the absence of evidence is not the evidence of absence, so it's theoretically possible two words could be juncture-based minimal pairs.







            share|improve this answer












            share|improve this answer



            share|improve this answer










            answered 2 days ago









            TaliesinMerlinTaliesinMerlin

            5,9361127




            5,9361127

























                2














                If you include compound words in your definition of "lexical word", this should be possible in theory, but I can't think of a specific actual example.



                Here's a hypothetical example: you could have a pair like sauce.pan vs. "saw.span", where the first would tend to have a phonetically shorter vowel in the initial syllable, while the second would have a less aspirated plosive in the onset of the final syllable.



                In John Wells' article on "Syllabification and allophony", he gives the near-minimal pair of selfish vs. shellfish, and argues that the first is self.ish while the second is shell.fish. Aside from the difference in the first consonant, I think that some people might argue that "selfish" is actually syllabified as "sel.fish" and that the difference in pronunciation relative to "shell.fish" is due to the absence of tertiary stress on the last syllable (e.g. ˈshellˌfish vs. ˈsel.fish). Wells doesn't think that tertiary stress is necessary, but other linguists do make use of the concept. That's why I think the clearest example would consist of a pair of compound words. Wells also mentions the near-minimal pair of "toe-strap" vs. "toast-rack".



                Some theories have more restrictive rules about syllabification



                Wells' conception of syllabification isn't universally accepted. In some theories, the rule of "maximizing the onset" of a syllable is treated as a generally applicable rule, even across morpheme boundaries in inflected words (such as planning, which some linguists would syllabify as /ˈplæ.nɪŋ/, despite the fact that it is a form of the word plan /plæn/).



                In such theories, there are several possible ways to analyze the distinction between pairs like "coring" and "co-ring" mentioned by Chris H. (In some accents of American English, both of these words can be analyzed as having the vowel /o/ as the nucleus of the first syllable.)



                The answers by user340953 and TaliesinMerlin mention the concept of "juncture", which is basically the idea that word (or morpheme) boundaries correspond to some kind of phonological entity. My impression is that this is not a very popular term/concept in more recent theories; user6726's answer to this Linguistics SE question says that "the concept has fallen into desuetude in linguistics over the past half century".



                The concept that I've seen used more often in papers that I've come across that take the maximizing-onsets approach to syllabification is to appeal to a larger phonological unit than the syllable, the "foot". "Feet" are mostly related to stress; to simplify things, you can think of each stressed syllable in an English word as being the start of a "foot".



                We could say that despite containing the same syllables, /ko/ and /rɪŋ/, "coring" and "co-ring" contrast because the second syllable of "co-ring" is (or at least was originally) a foot, but the second syllable of "coring" is not a foot. (The exact division of words into feet seems to be somewhat controversial; I'm not sure whether it would be more better to analyze "coring" from "core" as (ko.rɪŋ), with a disyllabic Hσ foot, or as (ko)rɪŋ, with a monosyllabic H foot followed by an unfooted syllable. Feet are commonly said to be "binary at some level of analysis" (either bimoraic or bisyllabic) which permits (ko.rɪŋ), and I think that would be the more usual foot-based analysis, but I think some people may have proposed more restrictive definitions of feet.)






                share|improve this answer






























                  2














                  If you include compound words in your definition of "lexical word", this should be possible in theory, but I can't think of a specific actual example.



                  Here's a hypothetical example: you could have a pair like sauce.pan vs. "saw.span", where the first would tend to have a phonetically shorter vowel in the initial syllable, while the second would have a less aspirated plosive in the onset of the final syllable.



                  In John Wells' article on "Syllabification and allophony", he gives the near-minimal pair of selfish vs. shellfish, and argues that the first is self.ish while the second is shell.fish. Aside from the difference in the first consonant, I think that some people might argue that "selfish" is actually syllabified as "sel.fish" and that the difference in pronunciation relative to "shell.fish" is due to the absence of tertiary stress on the last syllable (e.g. ˈshellˌfish vs. ˈsel.fish). Wells doesn't think that tertiary stress is necessary, but other linguists do make use of the concept. That's why I think the clearest example would consist of a pair of compound words. Wells also mentions the near-minimal pair of "toe-strap" vs. "toast-rack".



                  Some theories have more restrictive rules about syllabification



                  Wells' conception of syllabification isn't universally accepted. In some theories, the rule of "maximizing the onset" of a syllable is treated as a generally applicable rule, even across morpheme boundaries in inflected words (such as planning, which some linguists would syllabify as /ˈplæ.nɪŋ/, despite the fact that it is a form of the word plan /plæn/).



                  In such theories, there are several possible ways to analyze the distinction between pairs like "coring" and "co-ring" mentioned by Chris H. (In some accents of American English, both of these words can be analyzed as having the vowel /o/ as the nucleus of the first syllable.)



                  The answers by user340953 and TaliesinMerlin mention the concept of "juncture", which is basically the idea that word (or morpheme) boundaries correspond to some kind of phonological entity. My impression is that this is not a very popular term/concept in more recent theories; user6726's answer to this Linguistics SE question says that "the concept has fallen into desuetude in linguistics over the past half century".



                  The concept that I've seen used more often in papers that I've come across that take the maximizing-onsets approach to syllabification is to appeal to a larger phonological unit than the syllable, the "foot". "Feet" are mostly related to stress; to simplify things, you can think of each stressed syllable in an English word as being the start of a "foot".



                  We could say that despite containing the same syllables, /ko/ and /rɪŋ/, "coring" and "co-ring" contrast because the second syllable of "co-ring" is (or at least was originally) a foot, but the second syllable of "coring" is not a foot. (The exact division of words into feet seems to be somewhat controversial; I'm not sure whether it would be more better to analyze "coring" from "core" as (ko.rɪŋ), with a disyllabic Hσ foot, or as (ko)rɪŋ, with a monosyllabic H foot followed by an unfooted syllable. Feet are commonly said to be "binary at some level of analysis" (either bimoraic or bisyllabic) which permits (ko.rɪŋ), and I think that would be the more usual foot-based analysis, but I think some people may have proposed more restrictive definitions of feet.)






                  share|improve this answer




























                    2












                    2








                    2







                    If you include compound words in your definition of "lexical word", this should be possible in theory, but I can't think of a specific actual example.



                    Here's a hypothetical example: you could have a pair like sauce.pan vs. "saw.span", where the first would tend to have a phonetically shorter vowel in the initial syllable, while the second would have a less aspirated plosive in the onset of the final syllable.



                    In John Wells' article on "Syllabification and allophony", he gives the near-minimal pair of selfish vs. shellfish, and argues that the first is self.ish while the second is shell.fish. Aside from the difference in the first consonant, I think that some people might argue that "selfish" is actually syllabified as "sel.fish" and that the difference in pronunciation relative to "shell.fish" is due to the absence of tertiary stress on the last syllable (e.g. ˈshellˌfish vs. ˈsel.fish). Wells doesn't think that tertiary stress is necessary, but other linguists do make use of the concept. That's why I think the clearest example would consist of a pair of compound words. Wells also mentions the near-minimal pair of "toe-strap" vs. "toast-rack".



                    Some theories have more restrictive rules about syllabification



                    Wells' conception of syllabification isn't universally accepted. In some theories, the rule of "maximizing the onset" of a syllable is treated as a generally applicable rule, even across morpheme boundaries in inflected words (such as planning, which some linguists would syllabify as /ˈplæ.nɪŋ/, despite the fact that it is a form of the word plan /plæn/).



                    In such theories, there are several possible ways to analyze the distinction between pairs like "coring" and "co-ring" mentioned by Chris H. (In some accents of American English, both of these words can be analyzed as having the vowel /o/ as the nucleus of the first syllable.)



                    The answers by user340953 and TaliesinMerlin mention the concept of "juncture", which is basically the idea that word (or morpheme) boundaries correspond to some kind of phonological entity. My impression is that this is not a very popular term/concept in more recent theories; user6726's answer to this Linguistics SE question says that "the concept has fallen into desuetude in linguistics over the past half century".



                    The concept that I've seen used more often in papers that I've come across that take the maximizing-onsets approach to syllabification is to appeal to a larger phonological unit than the syllable, the "foot". "Feet" are mostly related to stress; to simplify things, you can think of each stressed syllable in an English word as being the start of a "foot".



                    We could say that despite containing the same syllables, /ko/ and /rɪŋ/, "coring" and "co-ring" contrast because the second syllable of "co-ring" is (or at least was originally) a foot, but the second syllable of "coring" is not a foot. (The exact division of words into feet seems to be somewhat controversial; I'm not sure whether it would be more better to analyze "coring" from "core" as (ko.rɪŋ), with a disyllabic Hσ foot, or as (ko)rɪŋ, with a monosyllabic H foot followed by an unfooted syllable. Feet are commonly said to be "binary at some level of analysis" (either bimoraic or bisyllabic) which permits (ko.rɪŋ), and I think that would be the more usual foot-based analysis, but I think some people may have proposed more restrictive definitions of feet.)






                    share|improve this answer















                    If you include compound words in your definition of "lexical word", this should be possible in theory, but I can't think of a specific actual example.



                    Here's a hypothetical example: you could have a pair like sauce.pan vs. "saw.span", where the first would tend to have a phonetically shorter vowel in the initial syllable, while the second would have a less aspirated plosive in the onset of the final syllable.



                    In John Wells' article on "Syllabification and allophony", he gives the near-minimal pair of selfish vs. shellfish, and argues that the first is self.ish while the second is shell.fish. Aside from the difference in the first consonant, I think that some people might argue that "selfish" is actually syllabified as "sel.fish" and that the difference in pronunciation relative to "shell.fish" is due to the absence of tertiary stress on the last syllable (e.g. ˈshellˌfish vs. ˈsel.fish). Wells doesn't think that tertiary stress is necessary, but other linguists do make use of the concept. That's why I think the clearest example would consist of a pair of compound words. Wells also mentions the near-minimal pair of "toe-strap" vs. "toast-rack".



                    Some theories have more restrictive rules about syllabification



                    Wells' conception of syllabification isn't universally accepted. In some theories, the rule of "maximizing the onset" of a syllable is treated as a generally applicable rule, even across morpheme boundaries in inflected words (such as planning, which some linguists would syllabify as /ˈplæ.nɪŋ/, despite the fact that it is a form of the word plan /plæn/).



                    In such theories, there are several possible ways to analyze the distinction between pairs like "coring" and "co-ring" mentioned by Chris H. (In some accents of American English, both of these words can be analyzed as having the vowel /o/ as the nucleus of the first syllable.)



                    The answers by user340953 and TaliesinMerlin mention the concept of "juncture", which is basically the idea that word (or morpheme) boundaries correspond to some kind of phonological entity. My impression is that this is not a very popular term/concept in more recent theories; user6726's answer to this Linguistics SE question says that "the concept has fallen into desuetude in linguistics over the past half century".



                    The concept that I've seen used more often in papers that I've come across that take the maximizing-onsets approach to syllabification is to appeal to a larger phonological unit than the syllable, the "foot". "Feet" are mostly related to stress; to simplify things, you can think of each stressed syllable in an English word as being the start of a "foot".



                    We could say that despite containing the same syllables, /ko/ and /rɪŋ/, "coring" and "co-ring" contrast because the second syllable of "co-ring" is (or at least was originally) a foot, but the second syllable of "coring" is not a foot. (The exact division of words into feet seems to be somewhat controversial; I'm not sure whether it would be more better to analyze "coring" from "core" as (ko.rɪŋ), with a disyllabic Hσ foot, or as (ko)rɪŋ, with a monosyllabic H foot followed by an unfooted syllable. Feet are commonly said to be "binary at some level of analysis" (either bimoraic or bisyllabic) which permits (ko.rɪŋ), and I think that would be the more usual foot-based analysis, but I think some people may have proposed more restrictive definitions of feet.)







                    share|improve this answer














                    share|improve this answer



                    share|improve this answer








                    edited 2 days ago

























                    answered Mar 19 at 13:00









                    sumelicsumelic

                    49.9k8117225




                    49.9k8117225























                        1














                        There are homonyms where one word is formed from a stem+prefix and the other isn't. The stress is often different, or the first/prefix vowel sound. One of the closest, at least in some accents, is coring (wiktionary). The more familiar meaning is from core+ing: "cutting a core" or (n) the core so formed, but there's a mathematical meaning from co+ring: "The dual of a ring".






                        share|improve this answer
























                        • As you mentioned, the stress in such words is often different, so I think it isn't a conclusive minimal pair for syllabification. Coring vs. co-ring could be analyzed as /ˈko.rɪŋ/ vs. /ˈkoˌrɪŋ/.

                          – sumelic
                          2 days ago











                        • @sumelic yes, I think there's value in the approach but the example could be improved upon. I also have to take time to think about your transcription, as I'm no expert on the finer points.

                          – Chris H
                          2 days ago






                        • 2





                          The issue that I'm thinking of is mainly theoretical. Intuitively, I wouldn't say that the first syllable of "coring" (from "core") is /ko/, but I also wouldn't say that the first syllable of "planning" is /plæ/, or that the first syllable of "lemon" is /lɛ/. However, syllabifications like "le.mon" are fairly often given, and justified by reference to the principle of "maximizing the onset" of syllables.

                          – sumelic
                          2 days ago











                        • I mention this in my answer, but the problem is that the difference in sound between the two words goes beyond syllabification. The vowel shifts. I think your idea is quite good (prefixes and suffixes may well generate the subtle allophonic difference that would signal a shift in juncture), but we're in a different kind of minimal pair with coring.

                          – TaliesinMerlin
                          2 days ago













                        • @TaliesinMerlin: In certain varieties of American English, both "core" and "co-" can be analyzed as containing the same vowel phoneme, /o/.

                          – sumelic
                          2 days ago
















                        1














                        There are homonyms where one word is formed from a stem+prefix and the other isn't. The stress is often different, or the first/prefix vowel sound. One of the closest, at least in some accents, is coring (wiktionary). The more familiar meaning is from core+ing: "cutting a core" or (n) the core so formed, but there's a mathematical meaning from co+ring: "The dual of a ring".






                        share|improve this answer
























                        • As you mentioned, the stress in such words is often different, so I think it isn't a conclusive minimal pair for syllabification. Coring vs. co-ring could be analyzed as /ˈko.rɪŋ/ vs. /ˈkoˌrɪŋ/.

                          – sumelic
                          2 days ago











                        • @sumelic yes, I think there's value in the approach but the example could be improved upon. I also have to take time to think about your transcription, as I'm no expert on the finer points.

                          – Chris H
                          2 days ago






                        • 2





                          The issue that I'm thinking of is mainly theoretical. Intuitively, I wouldn't say that the first syllable of "coring" (from "core") is /ko/, but I also wouldn't say that the first syllable of "planning" is /plæ/, or that the first syllable of "lemon" is /lɛ/. However, syllabifications like "le.mon" are fairly often given, and justified by reference to the principle of "maximizing the onset" of syllables.

                          – sumelic
                          2 days ago











                        • I mention this in my answer, but the problem is that the difference in sound between the two words goes beyond syllabification. The vowel shifts. I think your idea is quite good (prefixes and suffixes may well generate the subtle allophonic difference that would signal a shift in juncture), but we're in a different kind of minimal pair with coring.

                          – TaliesinMerlin
                          2 days ago













                        • @TaliesinMerlin: In certain varieties of American English, both "core" and "co-" can be analyzed as containing the same vowel phoneme, /o/.

                          – sumelic
                          2 days ago














                        1












                        1








                        1







                        There are homonyms where one word is formed from a stem+prefix and the other isn't. The stress is often different, or the first/prefix vowel sound. One of the closest, at least in some accents, is coring (wiktionary). The more familiar meaning is from core+ing: "cutting a core" or (n) the core so formed, but there's a mathematical meaning from co+ring: "The dual of a ring".






                        share|improve this answer













                        There are homonyms where one word is formed from a stem+prefix and the other isn't. The stress is often different, or the first/prefix vowel sound. One of the closest, at least in some accents, is coring (wiktionary). The more familiar meaning is from core+ing: "cutting a core" or (n) the core so formed, but there's a mathematical meaning from co+ring: "The dual of a ring".







                        share|improve this answer












                        share|improve this answer



                        share|improve this answer










                        answered 2 days ago









                        Chris HChris H

                        17.8k43276




                        17.8k43276













                        • As you mentioned, the stress in such words is often different, so I think it isn't a conclusive minimal pair for syllabification. Coring vs. co-ring could be analyzed as /ˈko.rɪŋ/ vs. /ˈkoˌrɪŋ/.

                          – sumelic
                          2 days ago











                        • @sumelic yes, I think there's value in the approach but the example could be improved upon. I also have to take time to think about your transcription, as I'm no expert on the finer points.

                          – Chris H
                          2 days ago






                        • 2





                          The issue that I'm thinking of is mainly theoretical. Intuitively, I wouldn't say that the first syllable of "coring" (from "core") is /ko/, but I also wouldn't say that the first syllable of "planning" is /plæ/, or that the first syllable of "lemon" is /lɛ/. However, syllabifications like "le.mon" are fairly often given, and justified by reference to the principle of "maximizing the onset" of syllables.

                          – sumelic
                          2 days ago











                        • I mention this in my answer, but the problem is that the difference in sound between the two words goes beyond syllabification. The vowel shifts. I think your idea is quite good (prefixes and suffixes may well generate the subtle allophonic difference that would signal a shift in juncture), but we're in a different kind of minimal pair with coring.

                          – TaliesinMerlin
                          2 days ago













                        • @TaliesinMerlin: In certain varieties of American English, both "core" and "co-" can be analyzed as containing the same vowel phoneme, /o/.

                          – sumelic
                          2 days ago



















                        • As you mentioned, the stress in such words is often different, so I think it isn't a conclusive minimal pair for syllabification. Coring vs. co-ring could be analyzed as /ˈko.rɪŋ/ vs. /ˈkoˌrɪŋ/.

                          – sumelic
                          2 days ago











                        • @sumelic yes, I think there's value in the approach but the example could be improved upon. I also have to take time to think about your transcription, as I'm no expert on the finer points.

                          – Chris H
                          2 days ago






                        • 2





                          The issue that I'm thinking of is mainly theoretical. Intuitively, I wouldn't say that the first syllable of "coring" (from "core") is /ko/, but I also wouldn't say that the first syllable of "planning" is /plæ/, or that the first syllable of "lemon" is /lɛ/. However, syllabifications like "le.mon" are fairly often given, and justified by reference to the principle of "maximizing the onset" of syllables.

                          – sumelic
                          2 days ago











                        • I mention this in my answer, but the problem is that the difference in sound between the two words goes beyond syllabification. The vowel shifts. I think your idea is quite good (prefixes and suffixes may well generate the subtle allophonic difference that would signal a shift in juncture), but we're in a different kind of minimal pair with coring.

                          – TaliesinMerlin
                          2 days ago













                        • @TaliesinMerlin: In certain varieties of American English, both "core" and "co-" can be analyzed as containing the same vowel phoneme, /o/.

                          – sumelic
                          2 days ago

















                        As you mentioned, the stress in such words is often different, so I think it isn't a conclusive minimal pair for syllabification. Coring vs. co-ring could be analyzed as /ˈko.rɪŋ/ vs. /ˈkoˌrɪŋ/.

                        – sumelic
                        2 days ago





                        As you mentioned, the stress in such words is often different, so I think it isn't a conclusive minimal pair for syllabification. Coring vs. co-ring could be analyzed as /ˈko.rɪŋ/ vs. /ˈkoˌrɪŋ/.

                        – sumelic
                        2 days ago













                        @sumelic yes, I think there's value in the approach but the example could be improved upon. I also have to take time to think about your transcription, as I'm no expert on the finer points.

                        – Chris H
                        2 days ago





                        @sumelic yes, I think there's value in the approach but the example could be improved upon. I also have to take time to think about your transcription, as I'm no expert on the finer points.

                        – Chris H
                        2 days ago




                        2




                        2





                        The issue that I'm thinking of is mainly theoretical. Intuitively, I wouldn't say that the first syllable of "coring" (from "core") is /ko/, but I also wouldn't say that the first syllable of "planning" is /plæ/, or that the first syllable of "lemon" is /lɛ/. However, syllabifications like "le.mon" are fairly often given, and justified by reference to the principle of "maximizing the onset" of syllables.

                        – sumelic
                        2 days ago





                        The issue that I'm thinking of is mainly theoretical. Intuitively, I wouldn't say that the first syllable of "coring" (from "core") is /ko/, but I also wouldn't say that the first syllable of "planning" is /plæ/, or that the first syllable of "lemon" is /lɛ/. However, syllabifications like "le.mon" are fairly often given, and justified by reference to the principle of "maximizing the onset" of syllables.

                        – sumelic
                        2 days ago













                        I mention this in my answer, but the problem is that the difference in sound between the two words goes beyond syllabification. The vowel shifts. I think your idea is quite good (prefixes and suffixes may well generate the subtle allophonic difference that would signal a shift in juncture), but we're in a different kind of minimal pair with coring.

                        – TaliesinMerlin
                        2 days ago







                        I mention this in my answer, but the problem is that the difference in sound between the two words goes beyond syllabification. The vowel shifts. I think your idea is quite good (prefixes and suffixes may well generate the subtle allophonic difference that would signal a shift in juncture), but we're in a different kind of minimal pair with coring.

                        – TaliesinMerlin
                        2 days ago















                        @TaliesinMerlin: In certain varieties of American English, both "core" and "co-" can be analyzed as containing the same vowel phoneme, /o/.

                        – sumelic
                        2 days ago





                        @TaliesinMerlin: In certain varieties of American English, both "core" and "co-" can be analyzed as containing the same vowel phoneme, /o/.

                        – sumelic
                        2 days ago











                        0














                        I don't think it is possible in English, since, unless the actual phonemes themselves change, as discussed in other answers, then the words would be pronounced the same. This is because we tend to pronounce syllable boundaries according to the rules of phonotactics, not according to the morphemic boundaries.



                        So if nitrate, night rate and nigh trait were each one word, and if the phonemes themselves were the same in each case, then they would simply sound the same, despite three different morphemic boundaries:




                        nitr|ate

                        night|rate

                        nigh|trait




                        where | represents the morphemic boundary.



                        I think we have to have to go to a language like Irish, which does something you don't see in English, to get two words that differ in morphemic boundary, but the problem is they sound the same!



                        This isn't quite what you are looking for but I think it is the nearest you will get.



                        Some words than historically ended in an -n now cause the following word to modify instead. This is usually called "eclipsis" but nasalisation is the correct term for the grammatical process, with "eclipsis" referring to the orthographic convention. It is described here. For anyone with Welsh (or similar) it is the nasal mutation with the addition of a rather strange orthography.



                        So from ár (treated as from *árn) "our" and athair "father", we get ár nathair /aːr nahǝr/ "our father". Unfortnately, nathair also means "snake", so ár nathair can also mean "our snake", thus confusing everyone who has ever learnt the Lord's prayer in Irish. Thus nathair is a pair of homonymous words, caused by a movement in the morphemic boundary.



                        They sound the same and look the same, unless you use capitals, as in the Lord's Prayer in Irish, in which case you get




                        Ár nAthair vs Ár Nathair




                        I know this is not exactly what was asked for but I don't think you will find anything better. Of course it is as much to with orthographic convention as anything else. This is illustrated by switching to Scots Gaelic, where we get




                        Ar n-Athair vs Ar Nathair.







                        share|improve this answer




























                          0














                          I don't think it is possible in English, since, unless the actual phonemes themselves change, as discussed in other answers, then the words would be pronounced the same. This is because we tend to pronounce syllable boundaries according to the rules of phonotactics, not according to the morphemic boundaries.



                          So if nitrate, night rate and nigh trait were each one word, and if the phonemes themselves were the same in each case, then they would simply sound the same, despite three different morphemic boundaries:




                          nitr|ate

                          night|rate

                          nigh|trait




                          where | represents the morphemic boundary.



                          I think we have to have to go to a language like Irish, which does something you don't see in English, to get two words that differ in morphemic boundary, but the problem is they sound the same!



                          This isn't quite what you are looking for but I think it is the nearest you will get.



                          Some words than historically ended in an -n now cause the following word to modify instead. This is usually called "eclipsis" but nasalisation is the correct term for the grammatical process, with "eclipsis" referring to the orthographic convention. It is described here. For anyone with Welsh (or similar) it is the nasal mutation with the addition of a rather strange orthography.



                          So from ár (treated as from *árn) "our" and athair "father", we get ár nathair /aːr nahǝr/ "our father". Unfortnately, nathair also means "snake", so ár nathair can also mean "our snake", thus confusing everyone who has ever learnt the Lord's prayer in Irish. Thus nathair is a pair of homonymous words, caused by a movement in the morphemic boundary.



                          They sound the same and look the same, unless you use capitals, as in the Lord's Prayer in Irish, in which case you get




                          Ár nAthair vs Ár Nathair




                          I know this is not exactly what was asked for but I don't think you will find anything better. Of course it is as much to with orthographic convention as anything else. This is illustrated by switching to Scots Gaelic, where we get




                          Ar n-Athair vs Ar Nathair.







                          share|improve this answer


























                            0












                            0








                            0







                            I don't think it is possible in English, since, unless the actual phonemes themselves change, as discussed in other answers, then the words would be pronounced the same. This is because we tend to pronounce syllable boundaries according to the rules of phonotactics, not according to the morphemic boundaries.



                            So if nitrate, night rate and nigh trait were each one word, and if the phonemes themselves were the same in each case, then they would simply sound the same, despite three different morphemic boundaries:




                            nitr|ate

                            night|rate

                            nigh|trait




                            where | represents the morphemic boundary.



                            I think we have to have to go to a language like Irish, which does something you don't see in English, to get two words that differ in morphemic boundary, but the problem is they sound the same!



                            This isn't quite what you are looking for but I think it is the nearest you will get.



                            Some words than historically ended in an -n now cause the following word to modify instead. This is usually called "eclipsis" but nasalisation is the correct term for the grammatical process, with "eclipsis" referring to the orthographic convention. It is described here. For anyone with Welsh (or similar) it is the nasal mutation with the addition of a rather strange orthography.



                            So from ár (treated as from *árn) "our" and athair "father", we get ár nathair /aːr nahǝr/ "our father". Unfortnately, nathair also means "snake", so ár nathair can also mean "our snake", thus confusing everyone who has ever learnt the Lord's prayer in Irish. Thus nathair is a pair of homonymous words, caused by a movement in the morphemic boundary.



                            They sound the same and look the same, unless you use capitals, as in the Lord's Prayer in Irish, in which case you get




                            Ár nAthair vs Ár Nathair




                            I know this is not exactly what was asked for but I don't think you will find anything better. Of course it is as much to with orthographic convention as anything else. This is illustrated by switching to Scots Gaelic, where we get




                            Ar n-Athair vs Ar Nathair.







                            share|improve this answer













                            I don't think it is possible in English, since, unless the actual phonemes themselves change, as discussed in other answers, then the words would be pronounced the same. This is because we tend to pronounce syllable boundaries according to the rules of phonotactics, not according to the morphemic boundaries.



                            So if nitrate, night rate and nigh trait were each one word, and if the phonemes themselves were the same in each case, then they would simply sound the same, despite three different morphemic boundaries:




                            nitr|ate

                            night|rate

                            nigh|trait




                            where | represents the morphemic boundary.



                            I think we have to have to go to a language like Irish, which does something you don't see in English, to get two words that differ in morphemic boundary, but the problem is they sound the same!



                            This isn't quite what you are looking for but I think it is the nearest you will get.



                            Some words than historically ended in an -n now cause the following word to modify instead. This is usually called "eclipsis" but nasalisation is the correct term for the grammatical process, with "eclipsis" referring to the orthographic convention. It is described here. For anyone with Welsh (or similar) it is the nasal mutation with the addition of a rather strange orthography.



                            So from ár (treated as from *árn) "our" and athair "father", we get ár nathair /aːr nahǝr/ "our father". Unfortnately, nathair also means "snake", so ár nathair can also mean "our snake", thus confusing everyone who has ever learnt the Lord's prayer in Irish. Thus nathair is a pair of homonymous words, caused by a movement in the morphemic boundary.



                            They sound the same and look the same, unless you use capitals, as in the Lord's Prayer in Irish, in which case you get




                            Ár nAthair vs Ár Nathair




                            I know this is not exactly what was asked for but I don't think you will find anything better. Of course it is as much to with orthographic convention as anything else. This is illustrated by switching to Scots Gaelic, where we get




                            Ar n-Athair vs Ar Nathair.








                            share|improve this answer












                            share|improve this answer



                            share|improve this answer










                            answered 2 days ago









                            David RobinsonDavid Robinson

                            2,537216




                            2,537216






























                                draft saved

                                draft discarded




















































                                Thanks for contributing an answer to English Language & Usage Stack Exchange!


                                • Please be sure to answer the question. Provide details and share your research!

                                But avoid



                                • Asking for help, clarification, or responding to other answers.

                                • Making statements based on opinion; back them up with references or personal experience.


                                To learn more, see our tips on writing great answers.




                                draft saved


                                draft discarded














                                StackExchange.ready(
                                function () {
                                StackExchange.openid.initPostLogin('.new-post-login', 'https%3a%2f%2fenglish.stackexchange.com%2fquestions%2f490330%2fenglish-minimal-pair-words-by-syllabification%23new-answer', 'question_page');
                                }
                                );

                                Post as a guest















                                Required, but never shown





















































                                Required, but never shown














                                Required, but never shown












                                Required, but never shown







                                Required, but never shown

































                                Required, but never shown














                                Required, but never shown












                                Required, but never shown







                                Required, but never shown







                                Popular posts from this blog

                                He _____ here since 1970 . Answer needed [closed]What does “since he was so high” mean?Meaning of “catch birds for”?How do I ensure “since” takes the meaning I want?“Who cares here” meaningWhat does “right round toward” mean?the time tense (had now been detected)What does the phrase “ring around the roses” mean here?Correct usage of “visited upon”Meaning of “foiled rail sabotage bid”It was the third time I had gone to Rome or It is the third time I had been to Rome

                                Bunad

                                Færeyskur hestur Heimild | Tengill | Tilvísanir | LeiðsagnarvalRossið - síða um færeyska hrossið á færeyskuGott ár hjá færeyska hestinum